Maintaining Problem Behavior in the School Setting

Summary of the attached Behavior Analytic Journal Article

Be sure to type your answers under each question.

Don't use plagiarized sources. Get Your Custom Essay on
Maintaining Problem Behavior in the School Setting
Just from $10/Page
Order Essay

Author(s)’ last name(s) followed by first initials: 

Date: Article Title: 

Name of Journal:
Volume: Pages:

I)  Introduction

a) List the main reasons why the authors decided to conduct this study. How did they justify

what they did? (At least 1 paragraph) (2 points):

b) Write the specific questions the author(s) asked or the hypothesis they wanted to test (usually found in the last paragraph of the introduction). Be sure to write it as a question (i.e- What will happen to the tantrum behavior when the DRO treatment plan is implemented) or in “If/Then” statement (i.e- If the DRO treatment is implemented, it is anticipated that the tantrum behavior of the children will decrease). (1 points)

c) How does this article interest you? How does it relate to the teaching you plan to do? (At least 1 paragraph) (1 point)

Methods.  Precisely describe.

a) the participants, including personnel, students, others (2 points)

b) The setting (location, physical arrangements) (2 points)

c) What was measured? (Usually particular behaviors) (2 points)

d) How did the researchers conduct their measurement? Did they use instruments, score sheets etc.?) (2 points)

e) What did the authors do to convince readers the measures were
reliable or dependable: (1 point) valid (measured what they were supposed to) (1 point):

f) Describe the procedures. (Who did whathow, with whomwhere, when, how often, and for how long?).  (At least 2 paragraphs) (3 point)

III. Results

Say what happened as a result of the methods being used. (You may wish to use the attached templates to sketch the results.) (At least 2 paragraphs) (2 points)

IV. Discussion and Conclusion.

a)List the main issues the authors discussed (At least 2 paragraphs) (2 points)

b) Refer back to the specific question the authors asked and indicate their answer (conclusion.) (1 point)

V. Your own comments. (i.e- How will you apply what you learned? What are your experiences? What was your favorite point, etc.) (At least 3 paragraphs) (3 points)

 

 

Escape-to-Attention as a Potential Variable for Maintaining Problem Behavior in the School Setting

Jana M. Sarno and Heather E. Sterling The University of Southern Mississippi

Michael M. Mueller Southern Behavioral Group

Brad Dufrene, Daniel H. Tingstrom, and D. Joe Olmi The University of Southern Mississippi

Abstract. Mueller, Sterling-Turner, and Moore (2005) reported a novel escape- to-attention (ETA) functional analysis condition in a school setting with one child. The current study replicates Mueller et al.’s functional analysis procedures with three elementary school-age boys referred for problem behavior. Functional analysis verified the participant’s problem behavior was maintained by escape from academic demands. Follow-up functional analyses in which target behaviors in escape versus ETA conditions were compared resulted in higher levels of target behavior in the ETA condition for 2 of the 3 participants. The current study also extended previous research by including a treatment analysis. Treatments de- signed to address escape and attention functions were more effective at reducing the target behaviors than treatments designed to target escape alone for all 3 participants. Results and implications for future research are discussed.

Incorporating experimental analyses into a functional behavioral assessment is an effective and time-efficient approach for the assessment and treatment of problem behavior (Hanley, Iwata, & McCord, 2003; Mueller, Sterling-Turner, & Moore, 2005; Mueller, Nkosi, & Hine, in press). The functional anal- ysis methodology developed by Iwata, Dorsey, Slifer, Bauman, and Richman (1982) is an analogue evaluation of problem behavior in which purported reinforcers are withheld and then delivered contingent upon target behav-

ior. In their original work, Iwata and col- leagues measured levels of target behaviors during experimental conditions (i.e., attention, escape, alone) and compared the data to levels of target behavior in a control condition in which the reinforcers were available noncon- tingently. Iwata et al.’s methodology has been used extensively to identify the behavioral function of self-injurious behavior in clinical settings and has been used with a variety of behaviors and in other nonclinical settings. Although use of functional analysis proce-

This article was taken, in part, from the first author’s thesis.

Correspondence regarding this article should be addressed to Heather E. Sterling, The University of Southern Mississippi, 118 College Drive, #5025, Hattiesburg, MS 39406; E-mail: [email protected]

Copyright 2011 by the National Association of School Psychologists, ISSN 0279-6015

School Psychology Review, 2011, Volume 40, No. 1, pp. 57–71

57

 

 

dures is reported less commonly in school settings (Hanley et al., 2003), studies have been reported with examples of disruptive school-based behaviors reinforced by peer at- tention (e.g., Broussard & Northup, 1997), teacher attention, (e.g., Gunter, Jack, Shores, Carrell, & Flowers, 1993), access to tangible items (e.g., Moore, Mueller, Dubard, Roberts, & Sterling-Turner, 2002), and escape from academic demands (e.g., Broussard & Nor- thup, 1995).

Although a functional behavioral assess- ment, including experimental analysis, may not be necessary to address all disruptive be- haviors in school settings (Gresham et al., 2004), additional research on the effect of idiosyncratic variables is needed. In school settings, as in other nonclinical settings, unique environmental variables (e.g., setting, personnel, physical) could require modifica- tions to the standard functional analysis con- ditions typically reported. For example, in school settings, tasks in the form of academic demands (e.g., ongoing instruction, indepen- dent practice worksheets) are, at least theoret- ically, present throughout the majority of the day. Likewise, concurrent and potentially competing reinforcers in the form of peer at- tention, teacher attention, or preferred activi- ties (e.g., reading a more desirable book) or items (e.g., playing with a toy hidden in a desk) for inappropriate behavior may be pres- ent. Thus, students may be provided with es- cape from academic demands, while subse- quently being provided with an additional re- inforcer for problem behavior. Because student behavior can be under the discrimina- tive control of multiple antecedent events or reinforced by multiple variables (e.g., teacher and peer attention, access to preferred materials, breaks from work), it is important to examine a combination of factors that may be maintaining problem behavior in the classroom.

Over the past few years, investigations of the effects of multiple variables have begun in and out of the classroom. For example, Hoff, Ervin, and Friman (2005) examined the separate and combined effects of escape and peer attention on disruptive behavior in the

general education classroom. Following a de- scriptive assessment, including interviews and direct observations, Hoff and colleagues for- mulated three hypotheses to test in an alter- nating treatments design: access to peer atten- tion, escape from a nonpreferred activity, and access to peer attention and escape from a nonpreferred activity. Treatment analysis data verified the initial hypothesis of access to peer attention and escape from academic demands. In addition, a combined intervention targeting both attention and escape decreased problem behaviors to near zero levels.

Moore, Mueller et al. (2002) investi- gated the influence of the simultaneous deliv- ery of therapist attention on self-injurious be- havior in a tangible condition. Following the initial functional analysis, attention in the tan- gible condition was evaluated using a reversal design. In one phase, juice and brief attention were delivered contingent on self-injurious be- havior. In the second phase, the delivery of the preferred stimulus (juice) was returned contin- gent on problem behavior and attention was withheld. The results of the follow-up analysis demonstrated that self-injurious behavior oc- curred at higher rates when the juice and at- tention were delivered concurrently than when the juice was presented alone.

By incorporating procedural variations in the functional analysis methodology, Moore, Mueller et al. (2002) demonstrated that the presence of attention could confound the outcomes of functional analysis condi- tions. Moore and colleagues hypothesized that “practical solutions for the tangible condition might be to restrict attention as much as pos- sible or to weaken the dependency between problem behavior and therapist attention by delivering attention on a response-independent schedule” (p. 284). However, Moore, Mueller et al. did not present treatment data to support their hypothesis. It is conceivable, though, as the authors suggested, that the influence of attention might affect other consequent analy- sis conditions.

In Mann and Mueller (2009), the func- tional analysis results of a girl’s aggression appeared to be maintained by attention. The results of the functional analysis of her behav-

School Psychology Review, 2011, Volume 40, No. 1

58

 

 

ior showed high levels of aggression in the attention condition and low levels in an escape from academic demand, access to tangibles, and a toy play control. When she failed to acquire a functional communicative response to replace aggression for attention, a follow-up functional analysis was used to evaluate whether access to attention was part of a chain of reinforcers maintaining aggression. In the follow-up analysis, attention-to-tangibles, at- tention-to-escape, and attention alone as a control condition were each used. Aggression was high in the attention-to-tangibles condi- tion and low in the attention-to-escape and the attention-only control. When functional com- munication training was used to address the attention-to-tangibles (i.e., manding for access to tangibles and attention, rather than attention only), the response was acquired and the ag- gression decreased. These results highlight two issues relevant for school-based func- tional analysis. First, Iwata et al.’s (1982) methodology is useful, even if structural vari- ants to assess multiple reinforcers are re- quired. Second, for behaviors maintained by multiple reinforcers, matching treatments to both reinforcers may be required to reduce target behaviors substantially.

Mueller et al. (2005) provided pilot data for an ETA condition used in a school setting. For one child, a functional analysis with an escape, attention, and toy play conditions was conducted using the procedures described by Iwata et al. (1982). Results of initial functional analysis showed that problem behavior only occurred in the escape from academic demand condition, however, lower than that typically was observed in the classroom setting. After escape was identified by the initial functional analysis, the researchers assessed a combina- tion of variables to determine whether differ- ential levels of problem behavior would occur with the addition of attention during the break from academic demands, as this was observed in the direct behavioral observation prior to the initial functional analysis. In the follow-up functional analysis, the escape-only, ETA, and control conditions were presented. A substan- tially higher level of tantrums was demon-

strated in the ETA condition than in the es- cape-alone condition or the control conditions.

Mueller et al. (2005) hypothesized that without the information derived from the fol- low-up analysis, an intervention based on the escape-only hypothesis would have failed. Al- though the results of Mann and Mueller (2009) provide some support for this hypothesis, Mu- eller et al. (2005) did not provide any inter- vention data. Other limitations should be ad- dressed as well. First, the investigation was a pilot study of the ETA condition and involved only one participant. Another limitation was that the consultant collected all data and, be- cause of staffing issues, no interobserver agreement data (IOA) were collected.

Given the limitations of Mueller et al. (2005), the current study was undertaken with two primary goals. First, we replicated Muel- ler et al.’s ETA investigation with additional participants and in a more controlled manner, including IOA data and multiple behavioral observers, to determine whether the ETA function would emerge in additional partici- pants. The second goal was to extend Mueller et al. by evaluating two different behavioral interventions, one that presented an escape- only treatment and one that was matched to both functions (escape and teacher attention). We predicted that differential treatment results would emerge for students who showed higher levels of problem behavior in the ETA condi- tion, with stronger treatment effects favoring the combined treatment for children with an ETA function when compared to children with escape-maintained problem only.

Method

Participants and Setting

Three elementary school-age boys re- ferred for problem classroom behavior partic- ipated. All students were enrolled in public schools and were placed in general education classrooms in a rural Southeastern school dis- trict. Teacher and parental consent were se- cured for participation; participant names used hereafter are pseudonyms. Brandon was a 6-year-old Caucasian male enrolled in a gen- eral education first-grade classroom. Brandon

Escape-to-Attention as a Potential Variable

59

 

 

was diagnosed with attention deficit hyperac- tivity disorder (combined type) when he was 5-years-old and was prescribed a 10-mg dose patch of methylphenidate (Daytrana). Franklin and J’Marcus were 5-year-old African Amer- ican males enrolled in separate general educa- tion kindergarten classrooms. J’Marcus and Franklin had no medical diagnoses and were prescribed no medications.

All sessions were conducted in the par- ticipants’ classrooms during typically sched- uled activities that corresponded to teacher- reported times when problem behaviors were most frequent. The students’ classroom teach- ers implemented all functional analyses and treatment evaluation sessions.

Measures

Functional Assessment Informant Re- cord for Teachers (FAIR-T). The FAIR-T is an instrument administered to teachers to gen- erate hypotheses concerning the function of problem behavior (Edwards, 2002). The FAIR-T is designed with four components to achieve this purpose: (a) general referral in- formation, (b) identification and description of problem behavior, (c) potential antecedents for problem behavior, and (d) potential conse- quences that follow the problem behavior most frequently. Researchers have demon- strated that the hypotheses generated from in- formation gathered via the FAIR-T correspond with behavioral function identified in experi- mental analyses (e.g., Doggett, Edwards, Moore, Tingstrom, & Wilczynski, 2001; Du- frene, Doggett, Henington, & Watson, 2007).

Intervention Rating Profile-15 (IRP- 15). The Intervention Rating Profile-15 (IRP- 15; Martens, Witt, Elliott, & Darveaux, 1985) was used as a social validity measure of the treatment conditions. The IRP-15 is composed of 15 questions that the respondent rates on a Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (strongly dis- agree) to 6 (strongly agree). Ratings range from a total score of 15–90, where a total score above 52.50 represents a rating of “ac- ceptable” (Von Brock & Elliott, 1987). The IRP-15 has high reported internal consistency (Cronbach � � .98), and all items load on a

General Acceptability Factor (ranging from 0.82 to 0.95; Martens et al., 1985).

Problem behavior. Child problem be- havior served as the primary dependent vari- able and was reported as the percentage of intervals in which the behavior occurred. Problem behavior included: inappropriate vo- calizations (Brandon, J’Marcus, Franklin), which was defined as talking or yelling with- out teacher permission; elopement (J’Marcus), which was defined as any movement 1 m away from the teacher or teacher-designated area without permission; and banging on surfaces (Franklin), which was defined as throwing ac- ademic materials in a downward motion to the desk, and/or floor so that it made an audible sound on impact. Additional data were also collected for task engagement during the treat- ment evaluation phases. Task engagement was defined as the student’s eyes directed at work materials and/or manipulating objects associ- ated with the teacher command. Task engage- ment is presented as the percentage of inter- vals in which behavior was observed during a session. A 10-s partial interval recording sys- tem was used for all observations. All sessions were 10 min in length.

Procedures

First, functional behavioral assessments that included teacher interview and direct classroom observations were conducted to generate hypotheses of behavioral function. Second, functional analyses were used to ver- ify escape from task demands as the maintain- ing variable for referred behaviors. Third, fol- low-up functional analyses were used to in- vestigate the additive effects of attention delivered during the break from academic de- mands (i.e., ETA). Finally, two different treat- ments were compared to examine the effects on target behavior when an escape-only treat- ment was alternated with an intervention pack- age that targeted escape and attention.

Functional behavior assessment. Each teacher was administered the FAIR-T as a semistructured interview in order to define target behaviors and their immediate anteced-

School Psychology Review, 2011, Volume 40, No. 1

60

 

 

ent and consequent events. Next, direct behav- ioral observations were conducted in the stu- dents’ classrooms. The information obtained from the functional assessment was used to form hypotheses about potential behavior-re- inforcer relationships. Conditional probabili- ties (VanDerHeyden, Witt, & Gatti, 2001) were also calculated from the observational data to determine the temporal proximity of specific consequences and target behaviors. Conditional probabilities for each participant were as follows: Brandon—escape � 74%, teacher attention � 24%, peer attention � 0%; Franklin—escape � 66%, teacher attention � 25%; peer attention � 5%; J’Marcus—es- cape � 59%, teacher attention � 29%, peer attention � 9%. Descriptive data suggested escape from academic demands or teacher at- tention in the form of reprimands and redirec- tion to work might be reinforcing the target behaviors identified for each child. Thus, each child proceeded to the experimental phases of the study reported below.

Functional analysis. A hypothesis- driven (Repp, Felce, & Barton, 1988) func- tional analysis was used to identify the rein- forcers for problem behaviors. For each par- ticipant, escape from academic demands and teacher attention were tested. A play condition was included as an experimental control. Con- ditions were presented in a random order and results were evaluated using a multi-element design. All conditions were 10 min in dura- tion. A 2-min break was given between con- ditions. During the break, the student and teacher continued with the naturally occurring classroom activities (e.g., read a book, transi- tioned between activities). Students were not informed of changes in contingencies across sessions and different stimuli were used were used across conditions (e.g., academic work- sheets during demand conditions; leisure items during attention conditions). Each partici- pant’s teacher implemented the functional analysis and conducted between 2 and 4 ses- sions per day.

Control (play) condition. The student was provided free access to attention and pre-

ferred play materials available in the class- room. The teacher engaged in interactive play with the student and delivered attention at least every 30 s. No programmed conse- quences or demands were delivered for target behaviors.

Attention condition. The student was allowed unrestricted access to activities/items typically available in the classroom. The teacher interacted with the student until he was engaged in an activity. Next, the teacher re- moved herself from the activity, saying she needed to do work at her desk. Contingent on target behavior(s), the teacher delivered verbal attention in the form of reprimands or redirec- tion to work, consistent with verbalizations noted in the descriptive observations. Follow- ing the delivery of attention, the teacher re- turned to work and the student continued to have free access to preferred items.

Escape from academic demand con- dition. During the escape condition, the student was presented with work materials identified by the teacher as associated with problem behavior in the past. A graduated prompting (i.e., verbal, gestural, physical) se- quence was used to deliver academic de- mands. If problem behavior occurred, the teacher removed the academic demand and walked away from the student. No attention was provided to the student. Following the 30-s break period, the teacher returned to the student and delivered another demand and re- peated the procedure described above.

Follow-up functional analysis. Fol- lowing the initial functional analysis, a fol- low-up functional analysis was conducted to investigate the additive effects of attention during the escape condition. All conditions were 10 min in duration, and 2-min breaks were given between conditions. The teacher implemented between 2 and 4 experimental conditions per day. The escape from academic demands and control/play conditions were im- plemented in an identical manner as in the initial functional analysis.

Escape-to-Attention as a Potential Variable

61

 

 

Escape-to-attention condition. Dur- ing the ETA condition, the student was pre- sented with work materials identified by the teacher as associated with problem behavior in the past. A graduated prompting (i.e., verbal, gestural, physical) sequence was used to de- liver academic demands. Contingent on target behavior, the teacher removed the task mate- rials and provided verbal attention during the 30-s break. The quality of teacher attention during the escape break and the nature of teacher attention were based on information obtained in the descriptive assessment (i.e., reprimands, redirections, physical attention). During the 30-s break, the teacher continued to deliver attention to the student in the typical manner for that classroom (e.g., “You need to get back to work”, “I told you no screaming, you have to work.”). Following the 30-s break, the teacher represented the task and the prompting sequence continued.

Treatment evaluations. A treatment comparison was employed to evaluate the tar- get behavior under two different treatment types. One treatment, Escape Extinction, tar- geted escape only. The other treatment (Es- cape Extinction � Differential Reinforcement of Alternative Behaviors), targeted escape and teacher attention. The ETA treatment condi- tions were evaluated using a B/C/B/C design for Brandon and C/B/C designs for Franklin and J’Marcus.

Escape extinction (EE). The EE con- dition was identical to the escape condition from the functional analysis with the excep- tion that no break was delivered for target behavior. Difficult academic materials were presented using a graduated prompting se- quence. No attention was delivered for target behaviors.

Escape extinction � differential re- inforcement of alternative behaviors (EE�DRA). The EE�DRA condition was implemented identical to the EE condition with one exception. During EE�DRA phase, the schedule of attention was based on the descriptive data obtained during baseline ob-

servations. For Brandon and J’Marcus, atten- tion was delivered every 30 s contingent on demonstrating appropriate behavior. For Franklin, teacher attention was delivered ev- ery 15 s. In this phase, teacher attention con- sisted of descriptive praise for appropriate be- havior (i.e., task engagement; “Great job working.”) and/or physical attention (e.g., pats on the back). If problem behavior occurred when the interval elapsed, the interval was reset and the teacher did not deliver attention to the student. That is, problem behavior did not result in the delivery of teacher attention.

Interobserver Agreement

Two observers were assigned to one stu- dent: one observer served as the primary data collector and the other for IOA. Agreement coefficients were calculated by dividing the total number of agreements by the number of agreements plus disagreements and multiply- ing by 100. IOA data were collected across a minimum of 30% of sessions during all phases of the study. IOA data during the initial func- tional analysis were: Brandon, M � 95% (range � 85%–100%); Franklin, M � 96% (range � 92%–100%); and J’Marcus, M � 95% (range � 90%–100%). IOA data during the follow-up functional analysis were: Bran- don, M � 98% (range � 90%–100%); Frank- lin, M � 91% (range � 85%–100%); and J’Marcus, M � 98% (range � 95%–100%). IOA data during the treatment sessions were: Brandon, M � 96% (range � 90%–100%); Franklin, M � 92% (range � 84%–100%); and J’Marcus, M � 93% (range � 90%–97%).

Procedural and Treatment Integrity

All teachers were trained to implement to implement the functional analysis and treat- ment evaluation conditions, based on proce- dures outlined by Moore, Edwards et al. (2002). For all activities for which teachers were trained, a series of steps was created. Procedural integrity was calculated each ses- sion and by dividing the number of correctly implemented steps by the total number of steps for that condition. Procedural integrity

School Psychology Review, 2011, Volume 40, No. 1

62

 

 

data (see Appendix A) were collected during each functional analysis session and ranged from 90% to 100% across all teachers.

Procedural integrity for individual com- ponents of each treatment condition was cal- culated for a minimum of 50% of the treat- ment evaluation sessions. During the EE con- dition, treatment integrity averaged 98% (range � 96%–100%) for Brandon’s teacher, 80% (range � 65%–91%) for Franklin’s teacher, and 89% (range � 86%–92%) for J’Marcus’s teacher. During the EE � DRA treatment, treatment integrity averaged 98% (range � 92%–100%), 86% (range � 58%– 100%), and 95% (range � 92%–100%) for Brandon’s, Franklin’s, and J’Marcus’s teach- ers, respectively.

Results

Initial Functional Analysis

Initial functional analysis results are de- picted in Figure 1. Data supported the hypoth- esis that each participant’s behavior was rein- forced by escape from academic demands. Brandon (top panel) exhibited problem behav- ior in an average of 14% (range � 12%–19%) of the observed intervals during the escape condition. The mean percentage of intervals with problem behavior during the teacher at- tention condition was 0.67% (range � 0%– 2%). No problem behavior was observed dur- ing the control sessions. Franklin’s (middle panel) mean percentage of intervals with prob- lem behavior during the escape condition was 11.33% (range � 8%–16%) and less than 1% during the attention sessions (range � 0%– 2%). No problem behavior was observed dur- ing control sessions. The bottom panel of Fig- ure 1 depicts the results of the initial func- tional analysis for J’Marcus. The mean percentage of intervals containing problem be- havior during the escape condition was 32.67% (range � 29%–37%). No problem behavior was observed during control and at- tention conditions.

Follow-up Functional Analysis

The top panel of Figure 2 depicts the results of Brandon’s follow-up functional analysis. The mean percentage of intervals containing problem behavior during the es- cape condition was 10.5% (range � 7%– 15%), and no problem behavior occurred dur- ing the control condition. The mean percent- age of intervals with problem behavior in the ETA condition was 11.5% (range � 7%– 14%). The ETA condition resulted in slightly more problem behavior than the escape con- dition. However, given the substantial overlap of the level of behavior between the escape and ETA conditions, the addition of teacher attention during the escape interval did not produce differential levels of responding be- havior for Brandon across the two conditions.

As shown in the middle panel of Fig- ure 2, Franklin’s mean percentage of intervals

Figure 1. Percentage of intervals con- taining problem behavior during the initial functional analysis for Brandon (top panel), Franklin (middle panel), and J’Marcus (bottom panel).

Escape-to-Attention as a Potential Variable

63

 

 

with problem behavior during the escape con- dition was 11.33% (range � 7%–17%). Low levels of problem behavior occurred during the control condition (range � 0%–3%). Prob- lem behavior in the ETA condition occurred in an average of 36.67% (range � 31%–44%) of intervals. The high level of behavior in the ETA and low level of behavior in the other two conditions suggests that Franklin’s prob- lem behavior was reinforced by attention dur- ing the escape period.

The results of the follow-up functional analysis for J’Marcus are depicted in the bot- tom panel of Figure 2. The mean percentage of intervals with problem behavior during the escape condition was 23.33% (range � 22%– 25%), and no problem behavior occurred dur- ing the control condition. A substantial in- crease in problem behavior was observed during the ETA condition, (M � 46.67%;

range � 40%–58%), suggesting that J’Marcus’s problem behavior was reinforced by attention delivered during breaks from work.

ETA Treatment Evaluations

Brandon. The top panel of Figure 3 depicts the percentage of intervals with prob- lem behavior and task engagement observed during Brandon’s treatment evaluation. Dur- ing the first EE treatment phase, an increasing trend in Brandon’s problem behavior was ob- served. (M � 38.4%; range � 17%–60%). Following the implementation of the EE� DRA treatment condition, an immediate de- crease was observed in Brandon’s problem behavior. The mean percentage of intervals with problem behavior was 11.8% (range �

Figure 2. Percentage of intervals con- taining problem behavior during the modified functional analysis for Bran- don (top panel), Franklin (middle panel), and J’Marcus (bottom panel).

Figure 3. Percentage of intervals with problem behavior and task engage- ment for Brandon, (top panel), Frank- lin (middle panel), and J’Marcus (bot- tom panel) during the escape-to- attention treatment evaluations with escape extinction (EE) and escape ex- tinction � differential reinforcement of alternative behaviors (DRA).

School Psychology Review, 2011, Volume 40, No. 1

64

 

 

1%–23%) of intervals. When the EE treatment was reintroduced, Brandon’s problem behav- ior increased slightly to a mean percentage of intervals of 12.33% (range � 12%–13%). The level of problem behavior observed in the second EE phase was relatively stable but did not reach the levels observed in the first EE phase. Finally, the reintroduction of the com- bined treatment of EE�DRA produced stable, low levels of problem behavior (M � 4.5%; range � 2%–6%).

The top panel of Figure 3 also depicts Brandon’s task engagement data during the ETA treatment evaluation sessions. During the first EE treatment phase, Brandon’s task en- gagement had a decreasing trend as problem behavior increased. Brandon was engaged with task materials, on average, during 27.2% (range � 0%–58%) of the intervals. When the combined EE�DRA treatment was imple- mented, Brandon’s level of task engagement increased immediately and continued on an upward trend over the phase (M � 73%; range � 40%–95%). Following the reintro- duction of the EE treatment, Brandon’s mean task engagement was stabilized at 78.67% (range � 78%–80%) of the intervals. In the final EE�DRA treatment phase, Brandon’s task engagement increased slightly to a mean of 86.5% (range � 83%–95%).

Franklin. The results for Franklin’s ETA treatment evaluations are depicted in the middle panel of Figure 3. During the first EE�DRA treatment phase, Franklin’s prob- lem behavior decreased slightly across the phase (M � 9.17%; range � 2%–19%). When the EE treatment was implemented, a large and immediate increase in problem behavior was observed, averaging 46% (range � 38%– 45%) of the intervals. When the EE�DRA treatment was reintroduced, a large and imme- diate decrease was observed in Franklin’s problem behavior (M � 25.5%; range � 25%–26%).

Franklin engaged with academic mate- rials during an average of 77.67% (range � 65%–95%) of intervals during the first EE�DRA treatment evaluation phase. With the implementation of the EE treatment, a

large and immediate decrease was observed in Franklin’s task engagement (M � 47.33%; range � 42%–55%) of observed intervals. Fi- nally with the reintroduction of the EE�DRA, Franklin’s task engagement increased slightly to a mean of 61% (range � 52%–70%) of the observed intervals.

J’Marcus. The bottom panel of Figure 3 depicts J’Marcus’s ETA treatment evaluation results for problem behavior and task engage- ment. During the initial EE�DRA treatment phase, J’Marcus exhibited low levels (M � 15%; range � 10%–20%) of problem behav- ior with a decreasing trend across the phase. Following the implementation of the EE treat- ment phase, problem behavior showed an immediate increase and continued trending upward (M � 51%; range � 33%–65%). Finally, after the reimplementation of the EE�DRA treatment, an immediate and large decrease was observed in J’Marcus’s problem behavior. Low and stable levels of problem behavior were observed in the final EE�DRA treatment condition, with a mean level of 3.75% (range � 2%–5%).

During the EE�DRA treatment phase, J’Marcus was appropriately engaged with ac- ademic work materials during a mean of 78.67% (range � 73%–83%) of the in- tervals. During the EE treatment phase, J’Marcus’s task engagement decreased sharply over the phase with a mean percentage of 33.33% (range � 16%–62%) of intervals coded with problem behavior. When the EE�DRA treatment was reimplemented, an immediate increase in J’Marcus’s task engage- ment was observed, and behavior levels were stable throughout the phase (M � 97.5%; range � 94%–100%).

Treatment Acceptability

Each classroom teacher completed the IRP-15 at the conclusion of each treatment phase. Overall, all teachers rated the EE� DRA treatment condition as more acceptable than the EE treatment condition. For the EE�DRA condition, the total scores were 89 for all participants. For the EE condition, the following total scores were obtained: 71, 30,

Escape-to-Attention as a Potential Variable

65

 

 

and 16 for Brandon, Franklin, and J’Marcus’s teachers, respectively. Thus, two of the three teachers responded that the EE treatment was an “unacceptable” treatment evidenced by to- tal scores substantially lower than the tradi- tionally used cutoff score of 52.50.

Discussion

The purpose of the current investigation was to replicate and extend Mueller et al.’s (2005) case study of a novel functional anal- ysis condition designed to assess the additive effects of attention as a reinforcer during breaks from academic tasks. That is, the pres- ent study sought to determine whether the addition of teacher attention to an escape in- terval (i.e., ETA) would result in elevated lev- els of problem behavior when compared to a standard escape condition for 3 children. The second purpose of the study was to evaluate whether a treatment package that targeted es- cape and attention functions would reduce tar- get behavior better than a treatment targeting only escape.

The descriptive data from the functional assessment suggested that problem behavior led to escape from task demands for all par- ticipants. Classroom observations revealed that teachers also provided attention (e.g., rep- rimands, requests to return to work) when the students were not engaged in work; low levels of peer attention for problem behavior were observed. The results of the initial functional analyses verified that all three participant’s problem behavior was maintained by escape from task demands. The first research question evaluated whether the addition of teacher at- tention during the escape interval would pro- duce elevated levels of problem behavior when compared to an escape-only condition. The results supported Mueller et al.’s (2005) findings, as the follow-up functional analysis showed increases in problem behavior in the ETA condition for 2 of the 3 participants rel- ative to the escape-only and play/control con- dition. Responding during the escape-only and ETA functional analyses was similar for Brandon.

As hypothesized by Mueller et al. (2005), the findings that attention can rein- force problem behavior during a work task suggest that the presentation of task demands may motivate problem behavior reinforced by escape from an aversive task and by teacher attention. In the conditions described by Iwata et al. (1982), the only establishing operation for the assessment of attention as a reinforcer was the deprivation of attention. As seen in the current analyses, attention functioned as a re- inforcer in contexts other than those in which a child was being ignored.

The second research question investi- gated treatment implications for ETA-main- tained problem behavior. A treatment program designed to match the escape function only (i.e., EE) versus a treatment targeting escape with the addition of teacher attention (i.e., EE�DRA) were evaluated. Problem behavior decreased for all participants during the EE�DRA treatment, and a general increasing trend in problem behavior was found during the standard EE treatment. Likewise, concom- itant increases in task engagement and de- creases in problem behavior were observed across all participants. Thus, differential re- sponsiveness to treatment based on behavioral function was observed, although differences for Brandon were minimal by the end of treatment.

The present study adds to a growing literature base of studies investigating the ef- fect of multiple reinforcing variables delivered together (compound reinforcers) or in se- quential arrangement (chained reinforcers). Golonka et al. (2000) described the additive effects of escape to an enriched environment contingent upon appropriate behavior as treat- ment for problem behavior in a classroom. Although a compound or chained reinforcer was not used in their functional analysis, the treatment results supported the reductive ef- fects of contingent escape to an enriched en- vironment as more effective than contingent escape alone. Mueller et al. (2005) used Golonka et al.’s (2000) findings as the basis for their ETA condition. However, no treat- ment data were described in their one-partici- pant case study. The present results add to

School Psychology Review, 2011, Volume 40, No. 1

66

 

 

Mueller et al.’s (2005) findings by replicating effects across additional participants. In addi- tion, preliminary treatment findings showed that when a behavior is reinforced by ETA, providing attention combined with EE inter- vention was more effective than escape extinc- tion alone.

Although treatment phases were trun- cated for some participants, the treatment comparisons data provide some intriguing in- formation for future study. Immediate changes of a substantial magnitude were observed for all participants’ problem behavior during the initial EE�DRA condition, regardless of the ordering of treatments. In subsequent itera- tions of the treatment comparisons, problem behavior levels for J’Marcus and Franklin continued to show differential responding, with substantially lower levels observed in the combined treatment phase. Brandon, who did not exhibit differential responding during the modified ETA functional analysis, showed less substantial treatment differences across the treatment conditions in the latter treatment phase comparisons. It is possible that the ini- tial differences between the EE and EE�DRA treatments could reflect an extinction burst associated with the EE treatment, rather than an actual difference between the two treat- ments. It is also possible that the inclusion of prompts in the EE condition may have pro- vided students with preferred attention, there- fore making the condition functionally similar to the EE�DRA condition. However, given the observed differences between the two treatment conditions, the effects of prompts were likely minimal. Additional treatment comparisons may provide additional support for matching treatment programs to behavioral function, the ultimate goal behind conducting a functional behavioral assessment.

The simplest explanation of the current treatment results is that the EE�DRA treat- ment worked better than EE for Franklin and J’Marcus because EE�DRA addressed both the escape and the attention aspects of the compound function. For Brandon, although EE initially produced higher level of problem behavior, each treatment reduced the behavior to similar levels by the end of the treatment

evaluation. The explanation of Brandon’s out- comes is also straightforward, but different from the reasons why the EE�DRA worked so well with Franklin and J’Marcus. That is, the addition of a positive reinforcer into Bran- don’s demand context most likely reduced the aversiveness of the task and therefore reduced the motivation to demonstrate escape-main- tained behavior.

The benefits of using positive reinforce- ment techniques to reduce behaviors main- tained by escape have been used successfully for over 30 years. Carr, Newsom, and Binkoff (1980) first demonstrated that attenuating the aversiveness of task demand situations through the delivery of highly preferred edi- bles during work tasks reduced escape-main- tained problem behavior. Several studies fol- lowed replicating and supporting the aver- siveness-attenuating benefits of introducing preferred tangibles or food items into demand contexts (e.g., Fischer, Iwata, & Mazaleski, 1997; Mazaleski, Iwata, Vollmer, Zarcone, & Smith, 1993; Mueller, Edwards, & Trahant, 2003). Adding preferred tangibles to a demand context makes use of reinforcers not identified during the functional assessment. That is, the addition of positive reinforcers into demand contexts can reduce escape-maintained behav- ior by using noncontingent reinforcement with a functional or arbitrary reinforcer, or by pos- itively reinforcing behaviors such as task en- gagement or compliance through differential reinforcement of alternative behavior (DRA). The results of the present study apply this well-known concept to a treatment in which escape and attention functions required sup- port. By interjecting positive reinforcers into the demand context using DRA procedures, the aversiveness of the task was reduced through the provision of a functional reinforcer.

Data also were collected for treatment acceptability, which has not been commonly reported in previous FBA research (Ervin et al., 2001). In the present study, all three teach- ers rated the EE�DRA treatment as more acceptable treatment than the EE alone. Sur- prisingly, two of the three teachers responded that the EE treatment was an “unacceptable” treatment; as such, low ratings of treatment

Escape-to-Attention as a Potential Variable

67

 

 

acceptability are often not reported in the pub- lished literature. Teachers specifically re- ported that they strongly disagreed that the EE treatment was effective for changing problem behavior, acceptable to use in the classroom, and consistent with interventions they had used in the past. These ratings may have been influenced by the fact that teachers completed acceptability ratings post-treatment use, after they had experience implementing the two interventions and had seen graphed data sup- porting the relative effectiveness of the EE�DRA treatment to the EE treatment alone, a finding reported in analogue treatment acceptability research (e.g., Tingstrom, 1989; Tingstrom, McPhail, & Bolton, 1989; Von- Brock & Elliott, 1987). Likewise, the higher ratings for the combined treatment may have been influenced by the addition of differential reinforcement for task engagement (i.e., praise), as previous researchers have reported that reinforcement-based interventions are generally rated as more acceptable than pun- ishment-based interventions (e.g., Blampied & Kahan, 1992; Elliott, Witt, Galvin, & Peter- son, 1984). Although extinction-based proce- dures are not technically classified as punish- ment in the behavioral literature, the teachers may have perceived the treatment as such, and thus rated it less favorably vis-à-vis a treat- ment that included a richer schedule of reinforcement.

A few limitations in the current study are worth noting. Data collection was discon- tinued early for Franklin because of a 9-day suspension from school for fighting at end of the school year. Thus, only two data points were collected during the final treatment phase. As such, definitive information about the long-term effects of the EE�DRA treat- ment is unknown. Additional replications of treatment phases, counterbalancing treatment order across students with escape-only and ETA functions, extended data collection within phases, and follow-up data would strengthen the experimental design of such studies and add information about long-term treatment effects. Next, the current treatment comparisons, although successful in reducing target behavior, did not allow for a full anal-

ysis of the mechanisms underlying the change. As described above, multiple theoretical pos- tulates are reasonable and plausible. Future studies may seek to determine exactly which mechanisms of behavior change are at play to reduce the behaviors. For example, a study might compare the reductive effects of EE with those of noncontingent reinforcement with an arbitrary reinforcer such as preferred tangible items to discover whether the addition of the attention in the current study reduced target behavior because it was functionally related to the target behavior or whether it simply reduced the aversiveness of the tasks presented.

Notwithstanding the limitations, the present study provides additional support for translating FBA research into practice in the schools. One criticism of previous FBA re- search is that individuals with specialized training (e.g., researchers, behavior special- ists) conducted the functional analysis condi- tions. In the current investigation, the stu- dents’ teachers implemented all assessment and treatment conditions, albeit with supervi- sion and feedback. Another strength is that the functional analysis occurred during standard classroom activities, rather than an analogue setting. In addition, all participants were gen- eral education students, unlike the majority of FBA studies conducted with participants with disabilities (Ervin et al., 2001; Hanley et al., 2003; Hoff et al., 2005).

The present study is in line with previ- ous research documenting the importance of identifying idiosyncratic variables during FBAs, in an effort to develop the most effec- tive treatment plan (Hoff et al., 2005). The results also add to a growing literature of school-based studies investigating the effect of idiosyncratic variables on problem behavior. The findings provide a heuristic and experi- mental example for incorporating descriptive assessment information into the development of functional analysis conditions in the school setting. Similar to previous studies, the results show descriptive assessment information was vital to the precise identification of behavioral function (Galiatsatos & Graff, 2003; Lalli & Casey, 1996; Mueller et al., 2005; Richman &

School Psychology Review, 2011, Volume 40, No. 1

68

 

 

Hagopian, 1999; Tiger, Hanley, & Bessette, 2006). The ETA condition was designed spe- cifically to assess the relative contributions of escape from academic demands and the addi- tive effects of teacher attention following problem behavior, a consequent frequently ob- served in classroom settings (Kurtz et al., 2003; McKerchar & Thompson, 2004). The results suggest the ETA condition may have utility for FBAs and treatment planning in the school setting. In future research, researchers may wish to investigate other sources of rein- forcement that may occur during the escape interval (i.e., peer attention, access to preferred activities/items). Also, researchers should focus on additional ETA-based treatment options. Future researchers may wish to investigate the addition of teacher attention with other es- cape-maintained treatment options.

References

Blampied, N. M., Kahan, E. (1992). Acceptability of alternative punishments: A community survey. Behav- ior Modification, 16, 400–413.

Broussard, C. D., & Northup, J. (1995). An approach to functional assessment and analysis of disruptive be- havior in regular education classrooms. School Psy- chology Quarterly, 10, 154–164.

Broussard, C. D., & Northup, J. (1997). The use of func- tional analysis to develop peer interventions for dis- ruptive classroom behavior. School Psychology Quar- terly, 12, 65–76.

Carr, E. G., Newsom, C. D., & Binkoff, J. A. (1980). Escape as a factor in the aggressive behavior in two retarded children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analy- sis, 13, 101–117.

Doggett, R. A., Edwards, R. P., Moore, J. W., Tingstrom, D. H., & Wilczynski, S. M. (2001). An approach to functional assessment in general education classroom settings. School Psychology Review, 30, 313–328.

Dufrene, B. A., Doggett, R. A., Henington, C., & Watson, T. S. (2007). Functional assessment and intervention for disruptive classroom behaviors in preschool and Head Start classrooms. Journal of Behavioral Educa- tion, 16, 368–388.

Edwards, R. P. (2002). A tutorial for using the functional assessment informant record for teachers (FAIR-T). Proven Practice, 4, 31–33.

Elliott, S. N., Witt, J. C., Galvin, G. A., & Peterson, R. (1984). Acceptability of positive and reductive behav- ioral interventions: Factors that influence teachers’ de- cisions. Journal of School Psychology, 22, 353–360.

Ellis, J., & Magee, S. (2004). Modifications to basic functional analysis procedures in school settings: A selective review. Behavioral Interventions, 19, 205– 228.

Ervin, R. A., Radford, P. M., Bertsch, K., Piper, A. L., Ehrhardt, K. E., & Poling, A. (2001). A descriptive analysis and critique of the empirical literature on

school-based functional assessment. School Psychol- ogy Review, 30, 193–210.

Fischer, S. M., Iwata, B. A., & Mazaleski, J. A. (1997). Noncontingent delivery of arbitrary reinforcers as a treatment of self-injurious behavior. Journal of Ap- plied Behavior Analysis, 30, 239–249.

Galiatsatos, G. T., & Graff, R. B. (2003). Combining descriptive and functional analyses to assess and treat screaming. Behavioral Interventions, 18, 123–128.

Golonka, Z., Wacker, D., Berg, W., Derby, M., K., Hard- ing, J., & Peck, S. (2000). Effects of escape to alone versus escape to enriched environments on adaptive and aberrant behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 243–246.

Gresham, F. M., McIntyre, L. L., Olson-Tinker, H., Dol- stra, L., McLaughlin, V., & Van, M. (2004). Relevance of functional behavioral assessment research for school-based intervention and positive behavioral sup- port. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 25, 19– 37.

Gunter, P. L., Jack, S. L., Shores, R. E., Carrell, D. E., & Flowers, J. (1993). Lag sequential analysis as a tool for functional analysis of student disruptive behavior in classrooms. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Dis- orders, 1, 138–148.

Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & McCord, B. E. (2003). Functional analysis of problem behavior: A review. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 147–185.

Hoff, K. E., Ervin, R. A., & Friman, P. C. (2005). Refining functional behavior assessment: Analyzing the sepa- rate and combined effects of hypothesized controlling variables during ongoing classroom routines. School Psychology Review, 34, 45–57.

Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., & Richman, G. S. (1982). Toward a functional analysis of self-injury. Analysis and Intervention in Develop- mental Disabilities, 2, 3–20.

Iwata, B. A., Wallace, M. D., Kahng, S., Lindberg, J. S., Roscoe, E. M., Conners, J., et al. (2000). Skill acqui- sition in the implementation of functional analysis methodology. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 181–194.

Kurtz, P. F., Chin, M. D., Huete, J. M., Tarbox, R. S., O’Conner, J. T., Paclawskyj, T., et al. (2003). Func- tional analysis and treatment of self-injurious behavior in young children: A summary of 30 cases. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 205–219.

Lalli, J. S., & Casey, S. D. (1996). Treatment of multiply controlled problem behavior. Journal of Applied Be- havior Analysis, 29, 391–396.

Mann, A. J., & Mueller, M. M. (2009). False positive functional analysis results as a contributor of treatment failure during functional communication training. Ed- ucation and Treatment of Children, 32, 121–149.

Martens, B. K., Witt, J. C., Elliot, S. N., & Darveaux, D. X. (1985). Teacher judgments concerning the ac- ceptability of school-based interventions. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 16, 191–198.

Mazaleski, J. A., Iwata, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., Zarcone, J. R., & Smith, R. G. (1993). Analysis of the reinforce- ment and extinction components in DRO contingencies with self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 143–156.

McKerchar, P. M., & Thompson, R. H. (2004). A descrip- tive analysis of potential reinforcement contingencies

Escape-to-Attention as a Potential Variable

69

 

 

in the preschool classroom. Journal of Applied Behav- ior Analysis, 37, 431–443.

Moore, J. W., Edwards, R. P., Sterling-Turner, H. E., Riley, J., DuBard, M., & McGeorge, A. (2002). Teacher acquisition of functional analysis methodol- ogy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 73–77.

Moore, J. W., Mueller, M. M., Dubard, M., Roberts, D. S., Sterling-Turner, H. E. (2002). The influence of thera- pist attention on self-injury during a tangible condition. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 283–286.

Mueller, M. M., Edwards, R. P., & Trahant, D. (2003). Translating multiple assessment techniques into an in- tervention selection model for classrooms. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 563–573.

Mueller, M. M., Nkosi, A., & Hine, J. F. (in press). Functional analysis in public school settings: A review of 90 functional analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis.

Mueller, M. M., Sterling-Turner, H. E., & Moore, J. M. (2005). Towards developing a classroom-based func- tional analysis condition to assess escape-to-attention as a variable maintaining problem behavior. School Psychology Review, 34, 425–431.

Repp, A. C., Felce, D., & Barton, L. E. (1988). Basing the treatment of stereotypic and self-injurious behaviors on hypotheses of their causes. Journal of Applied Behav- ior Analysis, 21, 281–289.

Richman, D. M., & Hagopian, L. P. (1999). On the effects of “quality” of attention in the functional analysis of

destructive behavior. Research in Developmental Dis- abilities, 20, 51–62.

Tiger, J. H., Hanley, G. P., & Bessette, K. K. (2006). Incorporating descriptive assessment results into the design of a functional analysis: Case example involv- ing a preschooler’s handmouthing. Education & Treat- ment of Children, 29, 107–124.

Tingstrom, D. H. (1990). Acceptability of time-out: The influence of problem behavior severity, interventionist, and reported effectiveness. Journal of School Psychol- ogy, 28, 165–169.

Tingstrom, D. H., McPhail, R. L., & Bolton, A. B. (1989). Acceptability of alternative school-based interven- tions: The influence of reported effectiveness and age of target child. The Journal of Psychology, 123, 133– 140.

VanDerHeyden, A. M., Witt, J. C., & Gatti, S. (2001). Descriptive assessment method to reduce overall dis- ruptive behavior in a preschool classroom. School Psy- chology Review, 30(4), 548–567.

Von Brock, M. B., & Elliott, S. N. (1987). Influence of treatment effectiveness information on the acceptabil- ity of classroom interventions. Journal of School Psy- chology, 25, 131–144.

Date Received: August 27, 2009 Date Accepted: January 15, 2011

Action Editor: Tanya Eckert �

Jana Sarno, MA, BCBA, is a senior program supervisor with Coyne and Associates Education Corporation in San Diego, California, and a doctoral candidate at The Uni- versity of Southern Mississippi. Her areas of interest include early intervention with children diagnosed with autism, verbal behavior, and functional analysis.

Heather E. Sterling, PhD, is an associate professor of psychology and the director of training for the school psychology program at The University of Southern Mississippi. Her research areas include functional assessment and analysis, single-case research design, school-based consultation, and intellectual and developmental disabilities.

Michael Mueller, PhD, BCBA-D, received his doctorate in school psychology from The University of Southern Mississippi. He is currently the director of behavioral services for Southern Behavioral Group in Atlanta, Georgia.

Brad A. Dufrene, PhD, is an assistant professor and director of the School Psychology Service Center at The University of Southern Mississippi. His research interests include functional assessment and school-based consultation.

Daniel H. Tingstrom, PhD, is a professor of psychology and is affiliated with the school psychology program in the Department of Psychology at The University of Southern Mississippi. His research interests include applied behavior analysis, and the implemen- tation and evaluation of behavioral and academic interventions.

D. Joe Olmi, PhD, is a professor of psychology and departmental chairperson at The University of Southern Mississippi. His research interests include positive behavioral interventions and supports, compliance training, and school-based consultation.

School Psychology Review, 2011, Volume 40, No. 1

70

 

 

APPENDIX A PROCEDURAL INTEGRITY FOR ETA CONDITION

Student: Session:

Teacher: Date:

Observer: Condition: ETA

This form is used to assess the level of procedural integrity for each teacher-implemented functional analysis ETA condition. Record if the teacher behaviors were implemented as planned (Yes) or not implemented as planned (No) during each FA control condition.

YES NO N/A

1. Seats student at his/her desk or table

2. Teacher places academic materials on the student’s desk

3. Teacher provides verbal instructions to student to complete the academic work

4. Teacher waits 5 seconds for compliance

a. The student complies

i. Teacher provides descriptive praise

ii. Teacher moves to the next demand

b. The student does not comply

i. Teacher restates the instructions with verbal and gestural prompts

ii. Teacher waits 5 seconds for compliance

A. Student complies

1. Teacher provides descriptive praise

2. Teacher moves to the next demand

B. Student does not comply

1. Teacher restates the instructions and provides hand-over-hand guidance

5. Teacher does not respond to any other problem behavior

6. Contingent on problem behavior

a. Teacher removes task demand for 30 s

b. Teacher provides attention during escape period

Repeat steps 3–6 for each demand sequence

Escape-to-Attention as a Potential Variable

71

 

 

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.


Get Professional Assignment Help Cheaply

Buy Custom Essay

Are you busy and do not have time to handle your assignment? Are you scared that your paper will not make the grade? Do you have responsibilities that may hinder you from turning in your assignment on time? Are you tired and can barely handle your assignment? Are your grades inconsistent?

Whichever your reason is, it is valid! You can get professional academic help from our service at affordable rates. We have a team of professional academic writers who can handle all your assignments.

Why Choose Our Academic Writing Service?

  • Plagiarism free papers
  • Timely delivery
  • Any deadline
  • Skilled, Experienced Native English Writers
  • Subject-relevant academic writer
  • Adherence to paper instructions
  • Ability to tackle bulk assignments
  • Reasonable prices
  • 24/7 Customer Support
  • Get superb grades consistently

Online Academic Help With Different Subjects

Literature

Students barely have time to read. We got you! Have your literature essay or book review written without having the hassle of reading the book. You can get your literature paper custom-written for you by our literature specialists.

Finance

Do you struggle with finance? No need to torture yourself if finance is not your cup of tea. You can order your finance paper from our academic writing service and get 100% original work from competent finance experts.

Computer science

Computer science is a tough subject. Fortunately, our computer science experts are up to the match. No need to stress and have sleepless nights. Our academic writers will tackle all your computer science assignments and deliver them on time. Let us handle all your python, java, ruby, JavaScript, php , C+ assignments!

Psychology

While psychology may be an interesting subject, you may lack sufficient time to handle your assignments. Don’t despair; by using our academic writing service, you can be assured of perfect grades. Moreover, your grades will be consistent.

Engineering

Engineering is quite a demanding subject. Students face a lot of pressure and barely have enough time to do what they love to do. Our academic writing service got you covered! Our engineering specialists follow the paper instructions and ensure timely delivery of the paper.

Nursing

In the nursing course, you may have difficulties with literature reviews, annotated bibliographies, critical essays, and other assignments. Our nursing assignment writers will offer you professional nursing paper help at low prices.

Sociology

Truth be told, sociology papers can be quite exhausting. Our academic writing service relieves you of fatigue, pressure, and stress. You can relax and have peace of mind as our academic writers handle your sociology assignment.

Business

We take pride in having some of the best business writers in the industry. Our business writers have a lot of experience in the field. They are reliable, and you can be assured of a high-grade paper. They are able to handle business papers of any subject, length, deadline, and difficulty!

Statistics

We boast of having some of the most experienced statistics experts in the industry. Our statistics experts have diverse skills, expertise, and knowledge to handle any kind of assignment. They have access to all kinds of software to get your assignment done.

Law

Writing a law essay may prove to be an insurmountable obstacle, especially when you need to know the peculiarities of the legislative framework. Take advantage of our top-notch law specialists and get superb grades and 100% satisfaction.

What discipline/subjects do you deal in?

We have highlighted some of the most popular subjects we handle above. Those are just a tip of the iceberg. We deal in all academic disciplines since our writers are as diverse. They have been drawn from across all disciplines, and orders are assigned to those writers believed to be the best in the field. In a nutshell, there is no task we cannot handle; all you need to do is place your order with us. As long as your instructions are clear, just trust we shall deliver irrespective of the discipline.

Are your writers competent enough to handle my paper?

Our essay writers are graduates with bachelor's, masters, Ph.D., and doctorate degrees in various subjects. The minimum requirement to be an essay writer with our essay writing service is to have a college degree. All our academic writers have a minimum of two years of academic writing. We have a stringent recruitment process to ensure that we get only the most competent essay writers in the industry. We also ensure that the writers are handsomely compensated for their value. The majority of our writers are native English speakers. As such, the fluency of language and grammar is impeccable.

What if I don’t like the paper?

There is a very low likelihood that you won’t like the paper.

Reasons being:

  • When assigning your order, we match the paper’s discipline with the writer’s field/specialization. Since all our writers are graduates, we match the paper’s subject with the field the writer studied. For instance, if it’s a nursing paper, only a nursing graduate and writer will handle it. Furthermore, all our writers have academic writing experience and top-notch research skills.
  • We have a quality assurance that reviews the paper before it gets to you. As such, we ensure that you get a paper that meets the required standard and will most definitely make the grade.

In the event that you don’t like your paper:

  • The writer will revise the paper up to your pleasing. You have unlimited revisions. You simply need to highlight what specifically you don’t like about the paper, and the writer will make the amendments. The paper will be revised until you are satisfied. Revisions are free of charge
  • We will have a different writer write the paper from scratch.
  • Last resort, if the above does not work, we will refund your money.

Will the professor find out I didn’t write the paper myself?

Not at all. All papers are written from scratch. There is no way your tutor or instructor will realize that you did not write the paper yourself. In fact, we recommend using our assignment help services for consistent results.

What if the paper is plagiarized?

We check all papers for plagiarism before we submit them. We use powerful plagiarism checking software such as SafeAssign, LopesWrite, and Turnitin. We also upload the plagiarism report so that you can review it. We understand that plagiarism is academic suicide. We would not take the risk of submitting plagiarized work and jeopardize your academic journey. Furthermore, we do not sell or use prewritten papers, and each paper is written from scratch.

When will I get my paper?

You determine when you get the paper by setting the deadline when placing the order. All papers are delivered within the deadline. We are well aware that we operate in a time-sensitive industry. As such, we have laid out strategies to ensure that the client receives the paper on time and they never miss the deadline. We understand that papers that are submitted late have some points deducted. We do not want you to miss any points due to late submission. We work on beating deadlines by huge margins in order to ensure that you have ample time to review the paper before you submit it.

Will anyone find out that I used your services?

We have a privacy and confidentiality policy that guides our work. We NEVER share any customer information with third parties. Noone will ever know that you used our assignment help services. It’s only between you and us. We are bound by our policies to protect the customer’s identity and information. All your information, such as your names, phone number, email, order information, and so on, are protected. We have robust security systems that ensure that your data is protected. Hacking our systems is close to impossible, and it has never happened.

How our Assignment Help Service Works

1. Place an order

You fill all the paper instructions in the order form. Make sure you include all the helpful materials so that our academic writers can deliver the perfect paper. It will also help to eliminate unnecessary revisions.

2. Pay for the order

Proceed to pay for the paper so that it can be assigned to one of our expert academic writers. The paper subject is matched with the writer’s area of specialization.

3. Track the progress

You communicate with the writer and know about the progress of the paper. The client can ask the writer for drafts of the paper. The client can upload extra material and include additional instructions from the lecturer. Receive a paper.

4. Download the paper

The paper is sent to your email and uploaded to your personal account. You also get a plagiarism report attached to your paper.

smile and order essay GET A PERFECT SCORE!!! smile and order essay Buy Custom Essay


Place your order
(550 words)

Approximate price: $22

Calculate the price of your order

550 words
We'll send you the first draft for approval by September 11, 2018 at 10:52 AM
Total price:
$26
The price is based on these factors:
Academic level
Number of pages
Urgency
Basic features
  • Free title page and bibliography
  • Unlimited revisions
  • Plagiarism-free guarantee
  • Money-back guarantee
  • 24/7 support
On-demand options
  • Writer’s samples
  • Part-by-part delivery
  • Overnight delivery
  • Copies of used sources
  • Expert Proofreading
Paper format
  • 275 words per page
  • 12 pt Arial/Times New Roman
  • Double line spacing
  • Any citation style (APA, MLA, Chicago/Turabian, Harvard)

Our guarantees

Delivering a high-quality product at a reasonable price is not enough anymore.
That’s why we have developed 5 beneficial guarantees that will make your experience with our service enjoyable, easy, and safe.

Money-back guarantee

You have to be 100% sure of the quality of your product to give a money-back guarantee. This describes us perfectly. Make sure that this guarantee is totally transparent.

Read more

Zero-plagiarism guarantee

Each paper is composed from scratch, according to your instructions. It is then checked by our plagiarism-detection software. There is no gap where plagiarism could squeeze in.

Read more

Free-revision policy

Thanks to our free revisions, there is no way for you to be unsatisfied. We will work on your paper until you are completely happy with the result.

Read more

Privacy policy

Your email is safe, as we store it according to international data protection rules. Your bank details are secure, as we use only reliable payment systems.

Read more

Fair-cooperation guarantee

By sending us your money, you buy the service we provide. Check out our terms and conditions if you prefer business talks to be laid out in official language.

Read more
error: Content is protected !!
Open chat
1
Need assignment help? You can contact our live agent via WhatsApp using +1 718 717 2861

Feel free to ask questions, clarifications, or discounts available when placing an order.
  +1 718 717 2861           + 44 161 818 7126           [email protected]
  +1 718 717 2861         [email protected]